www.fgks.org   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

Academia.eduAcademia.edu
Sexual and Relationship Therapy, Vol 18, No. 2, 2003 Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire: an instrument to assess sexual dysfunctional beliefs as vulnerability factors to sexual problems PEDRO J. NOBRE1*, JOSÉ PINTO GOUVEIA2 & FRANCISCO ALLEN GOMES3 1 Universidade de Trás-os-Montes e Alto Douro, Miranda do Douro, Portugal, 2Faculdade de Psicologia e Ciências da Educação da Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal & 3 Hospitais da Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal The aim of the present article is to present a new instrument, specially developed to assess beliefs about sexuality that are supposed to be closely related with the development of sexual disorders. Using a cognitive theory perspective we hypothesized that sexual behaviour and its problems are in someway related to the way we think about sexuality, our beliefs and our expectations. Although some questionnaires of sexual attitude, information and beliefs already exist, there is, until now, no specific measure oriented to assess both male and female sexual dysfunctional beliefs related to aetiology. The Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire is a 40-item self-reported measure constituted by two versions (female and male) rated on a five point likert scale. Both male and female versions present satisfactory test – retest reliability (r = 0.73 and r = 0.80 respectively), and internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.93 for male and 0.81 for the female version). Studies of convergent validity show a relationship with other measures of sexual and more general beliefs, as well as with measures of sexual functioning. Discriminant validity studies support its capacity to discriminate functional from sexual dysfunctional subjects. It is expected that these measures could be useful in both clinical practice and educational programmes serving as an indicator of vulnerability to sexual dysfunction. ABSTRACT Introduction Beliefs are ideas that we have about ourselves, others, or the world, that guide the way we interpret events, influencing our behaviour and emotions. According to cognitive theory, there are two different levels of beliefs, a more nuclear and unconditional one, called core belief, and a more intermediate and conditional one * Now at the Center for Anxiety and Related Disorders, Department of Psychology, Boston University. Correspondence to: Pedro Nobre, Rua Amorim de Carvalho, 97, 4460 Senhora da Hora, Portugal. Tel: +351 22938 6958; Email: pedro.j.nobre@clix.pt ISSN 1468–1994 print/ISSN 1468-1479 online/03/020171-34 # British Association for Sexual and Relationship Therapy DOI: 10.1080/1468199031000061281 172 Pedro J. Nobre et al. (usually known as attitude or conditional belief). Core beliefs are usually self-beliefs (also called self-schemas) that function in a more automatic and tacit way, not being easily accessible to the conscience. On the other hand, conditional beliefs are less central, more accessible to conscience and usually presented in a if ... then basis (Beck, 1996). These conditional beliefs (also called conditional rules) stipulate the conditions for the activation of the core beliefs or schemas. Beck (1996) gives some examples of conditional rules associated with different psychopathological states: ‘If I mingle others, I will be rejected’ (social anxiety), ‘If I attempt to do anything, I will certainly fail at it’ (depression), ‘If I have an inexplicable sensation, it is a sign of a catastrophic internal danger’ (panic). Whenever any internal or external stimulus fulfils these conditions core cognitive schemas or core beliefs would be activated or enhanced: ‘I’m friendless, rejected’ (social anxiety), ‘I’m a failure, worthless’ (depression) or ‘I’m physically vulnerable’ (panic). Several instruments were developed to assess general beliefs presumably related to psychopathology: Schema Questionnaire (Young & Brown, 1989), Sociotropy – Autonomy Scale (Beck et al., 1983) and Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (Weissman & Beck, 1978). Studies based on these measures had been the basis of the creation of several well accepted theoretical cognitive models of psychological problems: depression (Beck et al., 1979); anxiety (Beck & Emery, 1985); relationship disorders (Beck, 1988); personality disorders (Beck & Freeman, 1990); substance abuse disorders (Beck et al., 1993) and hostility (Beck, 1999). It is our intention to apply this framework to the sexual health field. Cognitive conceptualizations of sexual problems are far lacking, and we hypothesized that a systematic study of these variables could help in shedding some light over the comprehension and treatment of sexual disorders. In fact, several clinical reports and theoretical works point to some recurrent beliefs as etiologic factors of sexual dysfunction. Religious beliefs and conservatism (Lo Piccolo & Friedman, 1988; Kaplan, 1979; Masters & Johnson, 1970), fear of intimacy and losing control (Hawton, 1985; Kaplan, 1979; Lo Piccolo & Friedman, 1988; Rosen & Leiblum, 1995, Lazarus, 1988), body-image beliefs (Lo Piccolo & Friedman, 1988; Rosen & Leiblum, 1995) and beliefs about the role of affection in sex (Tevlin & Leiblum, 1983) are among the most common cited etiologic causes of female sexual dysfunction; while high performance beliefs, beliefs about women’s sexual satisfaction, and sexual conservatism appear as the top listed causes of male sexual disorders (Zilbergeld, 1992; Hawton, 1985; Wincze & Barlow, 1997). However, despite the strong convergence of these theoretical formulations based on clinical observations, its empirical validity remains to be tested in a systematic basis, and assessment instruments designed to do so are lacking. Some studies have previously assessed similar concepts in the field. Baker & De Silva, (1988), using Zilbergeld’s (1983) myths about male sexuality, conclude that dysfunctional males present higher beliefs in myths than functional subjects. Andersen & Cyranowski (1994) and Andersen et al. (1999) developed the Sexual Self-Schema (male and female versions), a questionnaire to assess cognitive generalizations about ourselves as sexual subjects. They postulate that these sexual schemas were developed through life experiences and would guide sexual behaviour. In their studies, they show that this construct is somehow related to sexual dysfunction patterns. Adams et al. (1996) had Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 173 developed a questionnaire for assessing sexual beliefs and information (SBIQ) especially in ageing couples. They hypothesized in the same direction as Baker & De Silva (1988) that the higher the belief in sexual myths and erroneous beliefs, the more the tendency to develop a sexual disorder. Unfortunately they did not present any supporting results. These studies, although contributing to understanding sexual dysfunction using a cognitive framework, do not develop a consistent model of cognitive theory of sexual problems. Our goal is to fill that gap by developing a measure to assess both male and female beliefs about sexuality. Moreover, this study is also part of a more systemic research project developed to assess the role of cognitive – emotional variables in sexual functioning (Nobre, 1999). For this purpose, two other measures were also created to study different levels of cognitive interference: cognitive schemas (Nobre & Pinto-Gouveia, 2002b) in sexual context (in order to assess the relevance of schema activation in sexual functioning), and sexual modal questionnaire (Nobre & Pinto-Gouveia, 2002c) (assessing the interaction between sexual thoughts, emotions and sexual response). It was hypothesized that sexual beliefs would stipulate the conditions for the activation of the cognitive schemas in specific sexual unsuccessful experiences. Once activated, these cognitive schemas would elicit a systemic structure composed by thoughts, emotions and sexual response. For example, the sexual belief (a man who fails to obtain an erection is a failure) would facilitate the activation of negative self-schemas (I’m incompetent) whenever an erection difficulty occurs. This negative self-schema, once activated would elicit negative automatic thoughts (I’m not able to satisfy my partner, I will never be the same again) and negative emotions (sadness, disillusion, etc.), impairing the sexual response. Past research with erectile disorders seems to support this model (Nobre, 1997; Nobre & Pinto-Gouveia, 2000a). Method Participants and procedures A total of 360 participants (154 females and 206 males) were recruited from the general population with the help of community volunteers (demographic characteristics presented in Table I). The subjects answered the questionnaires anonymously and returned them by mail. This sample was used in most of the reliability and validity studies. A second sample was also collected in order to perform a discriminant analysis. A clinical group of 96 subjects (49 males and 47 females) from the sexology clinic of Coimbra’s University Hospital answered the questionnaire. Subjects diagnosed with sexual dysfunction, using DSM-IV criteria constitute this clinical group. Erectile disorder (70%) and premature ejaculation (25%) were the most common diagnostics in the male sample, while hypoactive sexual desire (38%), vaginismus (24%) and orgasmic disorders (20%) were the main female complaints. A control group was also collected from the community sample above presented. Subjects were selected in order to match the clinical group in age, marital status and education level. Also a screen on the sexual functioning was performed using the International Index of Erectile Function (Rosen et al., 1997) and the Female Sexual Function Index (Rosen et al., 2000) to eliminate those subjects presenting signs of sexual dysfunction. Detailed demographic data from both male and female clinical and control groups are presented in Table II. 174 Pedro J. Nobre et al. TABLE I. Demographic characteristics of the community sample Age M Min – Max SD Marital status Single Married Divorced Living together Education level 0 – 4 years 5 – 6 years 7 – 9 years 10 – 12 years 13 – 15 years 16 or more years Female (n = 154) Male (n = 206) 24.4 18 – 55 7.2 % 82.2 15.8 0.0 2.0 30.6 18 – 56 9.4 % 55.2 39.8 2.5 2.5 3.0 3.0 2.0 23.4 8.1 60.5 8.5 3.5 5.1 17.1 7.0 58.8 Materials In order to validate our instrument, we used several other reliable and valid questionnaires that measure sexual beliefs and information, or more general beliefs usually related to psychopathology. Thus, besides our Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire, we also used: the Sexual Beliefs and Information Questionnaire (SBIQ; Adams et al., 1996), the Sexual Self-schema Questionnaire (SSS; Andersen & Cyranowski, 1994; Andersen et al., 1999), the Sociotropy-Autonomy Scale (SAS; Beck et al., 1983) and the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (DAS; Weissman & Beck, 1978). In order to assess the relationship between our measures and sexual functioning we also used the International Index of Erectile Function (IIEF; Rosen et al., 1997) and the Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI; Rosen et al., 2000). Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire (SDBQ) The SDBQ is a 40-item questionnaire assessing a sort of specific stereotypes and beliefs presented in the clinical literature as predisposing factors to the development of the different male and female sexual dysfunctions. The questionnaire presents a male and a female version assessing specific gender related beliefs. The subjects are asked to identify the degree of concordance with 40 statements regarding diverse sexual issues (from 1-completely disagree to 5-completely agree). The psychometric characteristics as well as the factor structure and the total index and domain scores of the questionnaires will be presented later. Sexual Beliefs and Information Questionnaire (SBIQ) The Sexual Beliefs and Information Questionnaire is a measure developed by Adams et al. (1996) to assess sexual myths and lack of information about normal sexual Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 175 functioning. The questionnaire consists of 25 items rated in a true false or don’t know bases. Correct answers are scored as 1 and incorrect as 0, with the total score demonstrating knowledge about sex related issues. Psychometric studies conducted by the authors (Adams et al., 1996), indicated adequate test – retest reliability and internal consistency. Internal structure assessed by factor analysis showed five components: TABLE II. Demographic characteristics of the clinical and control groups Female Age M Min – Max SD Marital status Single Married Divorced Living together Education level 0 – 4 years5 – 6 years 7 – 9 years 10 – 12 years 13 – 15 years 16 or more years Clinical sample (n = 47) Control sample (n = 46) 28.7 19 – 50 6.7 % 63.8 25.5 4.3 6.4 29.2 18 – 48 8.6 % 60.9 28.2 0.0 6.5 10.6 10.6 6.4 31.9 10.6 29.8 10.9 10.9 6.5 34.8 4.3 32.6 Male Age M Min – Max SD Marital status Single Married Divorced Living together Education level 0 – 4 years5 – 6 years 7 – 9 years 10 – 12 years 13 – 15 years 16 or more years Clinical sample (n = 49) Control sample (n = 49) 43.0 18 – 67 14.4 % 26.5 61.2 2.1 10.2 42.7 18 – 56 15.7 % 22.4 71.4 2.0 4.1 38.8 24.5 12.2 22.4 0.0 2.0 36.0 8.0 22.0 30.0 0.0 4.0 176 Pedro J. Nobre et al. Time/Patience, Stress/Pressure, Ageing, Sexual Satisfaction, Miscellaneous/Basic Knowledge. A Portuguese version of the measure was developed by Nobre (2000a). Sexual Self-Schema (SSS) The Sexual Self-Schema questionnaire is a measure developed by Andersen & Cyranowski (1994, female version) and Andersen et al. (1999, male version). These measures intend to assess cognitive generalizations about sexual aspects of oneself, that are responsible for guiding sexual behaviour. Both versions present acceptable test – retest reliability, internal consistency and validity characteristics (Andersen & Cyranowski, 1994; Andersen et al., 1999). The male version is composed of 45 adjectives rated on a seven point likert scale (from 0 to 6 according to the degree of identification with each adjective). A factor analysis of the scale shows a three factor structure: Passionate – Loving, Powerful – Aggressive and Open-Minded – Liberal. The female version consists in of a 50-item scale also rated on a seven point likert scale (from 0 to 6). Factor analysis also showed a three factor structure: Passionate – Romantic, Open – Direct and Embarrassed – Conservative. A translation from the original measure to Portuguese was developed, and applied to a sample of 65 students. Cronbach’s alpha of 0.78 for the total scale support the internal consistency of the Portuguese version (Nobre, 2000b). Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS) The Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale is an instrument developed by Weissman & Beck (1978) to assess negative attitudes conceptualized as predisposing factors for psychopathology (mainly depression). The measure presents acceptable reliability and validity characteristics (Weissman & Beck, 1978). A Portuguese version of the scale was developed by PintoGouveia et al. (1987). Psychometric studies of the Portuguese version conducted with 404 subjects showed acceptable test – retest reliability (r = 0.69) and internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.84). A factorial analysis of the Portuguese version resulted in a seven factor structure: perfectionism, absolutist imperatives, dependence/independence, adaptive cognitions, high performance demands, adaptive coping and autonomy. The Sociotropy – Autonomy Scale (SAS) The Sociotropy – Autonomy scale (Beck et al., 1983) is a self-reported measure that assesses two overall dimensions conceptualized as related to psychopathology: sociotropy, a dimension characterized by a dependence on social supportive feedback, and autonomy, defined as the investment in achieving independent status and individual goals. The scale contains 60 statements rated in a five point likert scale (0 – 4), 30 of which are related with sociotropy and 30 with autonomy. The scale presents high internal reliability with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients between 0.83 and 0.90. Validity studies point to moderate to high correlations between the sociotropy scale and measures of interpersonal dependence (Barnett & Gotlib, 1988) and to some inconsistent results regarding correlations between the autonomy scale and measures Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 177 of achievement and independence (Clark & Beck, 1991). A recent factor analysis (Bieling et al., 2000) suggests a by-dimensional structure for each scale: fear of criticism and rejection and preference for affiliation in the Sociotropy Scale; independent goal attainment and sensitivity to other’s control in the Autonomy Scale. A Portuguese version of the scale was developed by Cardoso (1998). Psychometric studies conducted with a sample of 225 subjects supported the internal consistency of this version (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.87 for the Sociotropy scale and 0.74 for the autonomy scale). The International Index of Erectile Function (IIEF) The IIEF (Rosen et al., 1997) is a 15-item, brief, self-administered measure of erectile function, evaluating five domains: erectile function, orgasmic function, sexual desire, intercourse satisfaction and overall satisfaction. Psychometric studies supported the validity (significant mean difference scores between a clinical and a control group) and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.73 and higher and test – retest from r = 0.64 to r = 0.84) of the measure. Studies with clinical samples also demonstrated its sensitivity and specificity for detecting treatment related changes (Rosen et al., 1997). The Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI) The FSFI (Rosen et al., 2000) is a 19 item instrument, easily administered and scored, providing detailed information on the major dimensions of sexual function: sexual interest/desire, sexual arousal, lubrication, orgasm, sexual satisfaction and sexual pain. The measure presents acceptable test – retest reliability (r = 0.79 to r = 0.86), internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.82 and higher) and validity (demonstrated by significant mean difference scores between a clinical and a control group). Results Item analysis To develop the instruments outlined above, we proceeded with an item analysis of the initial Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire. The items were selected from an initial version of 95 item (female version) and 94 item (male version). From those, 49 items (male and female versions) were selected based on item-total correlation (r 4 0.40), and clinical relevance (correlations with FSFI and IIEF total scores— p 5 0.05). The remainder of the 49 items (both male and female versions) were submitted to a exploratory factor analysis where we rejected nine items from each version that presented factor loadings higher than 0.4 in more than one factor or which didn’t load significantly (higher than 0.4) in none of the factors. The remaining 40 items constitute the final version of the male and female SDBQ. 178 Pedro J. Nobre et al. Factor analysis and domain scores In order to analyse the internal structure of our instrument, we submitted our results to a factor analysis of both male and female versions as recommended by Campbell & Fiske (1959) and Cronbach & Meehl (1955). Female version A principal component analysis with varimax rotation was performed to investigate the internal structure of the 40 beliefs presented in the female version (Table III). Six factors were identified using Catell’s scree test. The factors identified showed theoretical consistency and account for 42.5% of the total variance (F1 = 17.0%, F2 = 6.1%, F3 = 5.3%, F4 = 5.2%, F5 = 4.6%, F6 = 4.3%). Kaiser – Meyer – Olkin of 0.77 supported the adequacy of the sample, and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (Chisquare = 2426, p 5 0.001). The item selection for each factor was based on statistical and interpretability criteria. Inclusion decision was based on loadings higher than 0.4 on the respective factor. Items which didn’t load high on any of the factors were excluded. Based on these criteria six items were excluded: 9—‘there is a variety of ways of getting pleasure and reaching orgasm’, 16—‘simultaneous orgasm for two partners is essential for a satisfying sexual encounter’, 19—‘A successful professional career implies control of sexual behaviour’, 21—‘men only pay attention to young, attractive women’, 25—‘any woman who initiates sexual activity is immoral’, 29—‘if women let themselves go sexually they are totally under men’s control’. Items 6, 7, 26 and 37 although presenting some high loadings in more than one factor were retained (included in the factors where the loadings were higher). The six domains identified were as follows: (1) Sexual conservatism: factor characterized by the idea that coitus is the central aspect of human sexuality, considering masturbation, oral and anal sex as deviant and sinful activities. Female’s sexual role as passive and receptive, with virginity being an important value for non-married women. (2) Sexual desire and pleasure as a sin: dimension dominated by the idea that sex is a male activity, where women must control their sexual urges and pleasure since these are sinful experiences. (3) Age related beliefs: domain where the central theme is the decrease of sexual desire, pleasure or orgasm with age, especially after menopause. (4) Body-image beliefs: factor characterized by the idea of body-image as a central aspect on female’s sexuality. (5) Affection primacy: dimension where affection, love and agreement between partners constitute the central aspect of human sexuality. Since the items were scored in reverse order, the higher the factor score the lower the affection importance. (6) Motherhood primacy: factor characterized by the idea that motherhood activities are the most important female pleasure and that procreation is the goal of any sexual experience. Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 179 Analysing the inter-correlation between the diverse dimensions of the female version we may highlight the overall high relationship showed by all the dimensions except one (denying affection primacy). In fact, all correlations showed statistical significance (p 5 0.01), with sexual conservatism presenting the higher correlations with the other factors. Affection primacy on the contrary does not correlate with any of the remaining domains, indicating that this factor is not clearly associated with the other concepts analysed by the questionnaire (Table IV). The range of possible domain and total scores for the Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire is presented in Table V. The higher the scores on the total scale the greater the dysfunctional beliefs. Male version To assess the internal structure of the male sexual dysfunctional beliefs questionnaire, we performed a factor analysis of the 40 item scale using a varimax rotation (Table VI). Six factors were identified using Catell’s sree test accounting for 49.4% of the total variance (F1 = 25.1%, F2 = 7.7%, F3 = 4.7%, F4 = 4.5%, F5 = 3.9%, F6 = 3.6%). The factors identified were theoretically sound, Kaiser – Meyer – Olkin of 0.85 supported the adequacy of the sample, and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (Chisquare = 2778.72, p 5 0.001). The item selection for each factor was based on statistical and interpretability criteria. Inclusion decision was based on loadings higher than 0.4 on the respective factor. Items which didn’t load highly on any of them were excluded. Based on these criteria three items were excluded: 14, ‘there are certain universal rules about what is normal during sexual activity’; 20, ‘a woman may stop loving a man if he is not capable of satisfying her sexually’; 23, ‘A successful career implies the control of sexual urges’. Items 6, 18, 21 and 39 although presenting some high loadings in more than one factor were retained (included in the factors where the loadings were higher). The six domains identified were the following: (1) Sexual conservatism – coitus/procreation primacy: dimension characterized by conservative ideas about sexual behavior. Sex before marriage is unacceptable, has to be quick, directed to coitus, without foreplay, with man on top and serving procreative goals. (2) Female sexual power – need for sexual control: domain closely related with the idea that female sexual power can be dangerous and if men don’t control their sexual urges, they will fall under women’s power. (3) ‘Macho’ belief: factor dominated by the concept of man’s capacity for being always ready for sex, satisfy all women, and keep an erected penis until the end of any sexual activity. (4) Beliefs about women’s sexual satisfaction: dimension characterized by the importance of satisfying female partners, and by the idea that penis erection and vaginal coitus are necessary in order to sexually satisfy any woman. (5) Restricted attitude toward sexual activity: factor where sexual fantasies, oral and anal sex are seen as unhealthy or incorrect experiences. 180 Pedro J. Nobre et al. TABLE III. Female SDBQ principal component analysis with varimax rotation (n = 154) Factors SDBQ items Sexual conservatism 2. Masturbation is wrong and sinful 4. The best gift woman could bring to marriage is her virginity 7. Masturbation is not a proper activity for respectable women 13. Reaching climax/orgasm is acceptable for men but not for women 14. Sexual activity must be initiated by man 17. Orgasm is possible only by vaginal intercourse 27. Sexual intercourse during menstruation can cause health problems 28. Oral sex is one of the biggest perversions 32. Anal sex is a perverted activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 0.66 0.63 7 0.08 7 0.05 0.14 0.25 0.14 0.20 7 0.02 7 0.08 0.15 7 0.10 0.53 0.20 0.23 0.41 7 0.04 7 0.09 0.43 7 0.11 0.27 0.14 0.27 0.02 0.45 0.19 0.22 0.01 0.01 0.32 0.46 0.15 7 0.03 0.27 0.11 0.28 0.40 0.33 7 0.03 7 0.03 0.17 0.12 0.64 0.35 0.05 7 0.02 0.12 0.29 0.65 0.06 0.07 7 0.04 7 0.08 0.08 7 0.07 0.08 0.50 0.79 0.09 0.09 0.29 0.13 0.20 7 0.00 0.21 0.02 0.13 0.72 7 0.02 0.03 7 0.01 0.28 0.05 0.63 0.14 0.22 0.04 7 0.37 0.06 0.52 0.27 0.46 0.02 0.26 0.23 0.45 0.13 0.28 7 0.01 0.10 0.17 0.11 0.67 0.09 7 0.07 0.02 0.11 0.47 0.52 7 0.04 0.13 7 0.09 Sexual desire and pleasure as a sin 15. 34. 35. 36. 37. 39. Sex is dirty and sinful Sex should happen only if a man initiates There is just one acceptable way of having sex (missionary position) Exper. pleasure during sexual act. is not acceptable in a virtuous woman A good mother must control her sexual urges A woman who only derives sex pleasure through clitoris stimulation is sick Age related beliefs 5. 6. After menopause women lose their sexual desire Women who have sexual fantasies are perverted (continued ) Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 181 TABLE III. (continued ) Factors SDBQ items 1 2 3 4 8. 0.15 0.19 0.70 0.11 7 0.01 0.07 0.01 7 0.18 0.54 0.22 7 0.05 0.23 0.17 0.18 0.62 0.17 7 0.12 0.26 0.14 0.03 0.34 0.64 7 0.08 7 0.05 0.01 0.12 0.25 0.71 0.02 0.27 0.12 0.24 0.04 0.53 7 0.06 0.05 0.24 0.21 0.10 0.48 7 0.04 7 0.12 0.06 7 0.06 0.24 7 0.38 0.52 7 0.21 0.09 0.06 0.11 0.04 0.56 7 0.03 0.28 0.11 0.16 0.21 0.47 0.00 0.01 0.14 0.05 0.07 0.67 0.08 0.25 0.08 7 0.00 0.14 0.56 0.04 0.23 7 0.01 0.05 7 0.07 0.60 7 0.05 0.15 0.16 7 0.19 0.40 0.12 0.54 0.14 0.12 0.23 0.14 0.02 0.60 0.19 0.00 0.23 0.02 7 0.16 0.53 After menopause women can’t reach orgasm 11. In the bedroom the man is the boss 20. As women age the pleasure they get from sex decreases 5 6 Body-image beliefs 10. 12. 38. 40. Women who are not physically attractive can’t be sexually satisfied A good mother can’t be sexually active An ugly woman is not capable of sexually satisfying her partner Pure girls don’t engage in sexual activity Affection primacy 1. 3. 18. 22. 23. 24. Love and affection from a partner are necessary for good sexa The most important component of sex is mutual affectiona The goal of sex is for men to be satisfied Sex is a beautiful and pure activitya Sex without love is like food without flavoura As long as both consent agree anything goesa Motherhood primacy 26. 30. 31. Sex is meant only for procreation Being nice and smiling at men can be dangerous The most wonderful emotions that a woman can experience are maternal (continued ) 182 Pedro J. Nobre et al. TABLE III. (continued ) Factors SDBQ items 1 2 3 33. 0.10 0.05 0.24 a In the bedroom the woman is the boss 4 7 0.22 5 6 0.12 0.43 F5 F6 – 0.01 – Items scored in reverse order TABLE IV. Female SDBQ domain intercorrelations (n = 154) Domains F1 Sexual conservatism F2 Sexual desire and pleasure as a sin F3 Age related beliefs F4 Body-image beliefs F5 Denying affection primacy F6 Motherhood primacy F1 F2 – 0.42** – 0.42** 0.43** 0.06 0.41** 0.43** 0.52** 0.09 0.27** F3 – 0.42** 7 0.08 0.38** F4 – 7 0.01 0.21** *p 5 0.05. **p 5 0.01. TABLE V. Domain and total scores of the SDBQ (female version) Domains Item numbers F1 Sexual conservatism F2 Sexual desire and pleasure as a sin F3 Age related beliefs F4 Body-image beliefs F5 Denying affection primacy F6 Motherhood primacy Total 2, 4, 7, 13, 14, 17, 27, 28, 32 15, 34, 35, 36, 37, 39 5, 6, 8, 11, 20 10, 12, 38, 40 1, 3, 18, 22, 23, 24 26, 30, 31, 33 – Minimum Maximum 9 6 5 4 6 4 34 45 30 25 20 30 20 170 *Items 1, 3, 22, 23, 24 are scored in reverse order. (6) Sex as an abuse of men’s power: dimension dominated by the idea of sex as an act of violation or abuse of woman’s body by male. The inter-correlations between the diverse factors of the male sexual beliefs questionnaire, present a consistent relationship (Table VII). All inter-correlations are greater than 0.34 and statistically significant (p 5 0.01). These results show that the scale assesses different dimensions of the same general concept. Looking more specifically to the individual relationship, we may highlight the higher correlations of Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 183 the sexual conservatism dimension with all the other factors, specially, sex as an abuse of men’s power (r = 0.61, p 5 0.01) and restrictive attitude toward sex (r = 0.59, p 5 0.01). The range of possible domain and total scores for the Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire (male version) is presented in Table VIII. The higher the scores on the total scale the greater the dysfunctional beliefs. Reliability studies In order to assess the reliability of our measure, we performed two types of tests: test – retest reliability to assess the temporal stability of the questionnaires and internal consistency to analyse the degree of consistency (relatedness) among the several dimensions represented in our instrument. Test – retest reliability Test – Retest reliability for both male and female versions was assessed by computing Pearson product – moment correlations between two consecutive administrations of the questionnaires with a four week interval. Both male and female versions presented statistically significant results (p 5 0.05) for the total scale (r = 0.73 and r = 0.80 respectively), showing that the instrument present good stability across time. However, when analysing specifically the results for each dimension of both male and female measures we found some non-significant correlations (Table IX). Internal consistency Internal consistency of the instrument was assessed by calculating Cronbach’s alpha statistic for the total scale and also for each dimension of both male and female versions (Table X). Results for the total scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.93 for the male and 0.81 for the female version) supported the high internal consistency of the questionnaires. When we analysed each dimension, a relative discrepancy was observed in its consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha statistic ranging between 0.50 and 0.89. The smallest results are presented from the motherhood primacy and denying affection primacy of the female version and from the restrictive attitude toward sex and sex as an abuse of men’s power of the male version. These findings may be interpreted as possible lack of consistency between the items within the mentioned dimensions. Validity studies Convergent validity. In order to assess the convergent validity of our measure we used self-reported questionnaires partially associated with the dimensions we assess. We performed Pearson product – moment correlations between our questionnaire and the SBIQ, DAS, SSS, IIEF and FSFI. 184 Pedro J. Nobre et al. TABLE VI. Male SDBQ principal component analysis with varimax rotation (n = 206) Factors SDBQ items Sexual conservatism 2. Orgasm is possible only by vaginal intercourse 5. Women have no other choice but to be sex. Subjugated by man’s power 9. A shorter duration of intercourse is a sign of man’s power 18. In sex anything but vaginal intercourse is unacceptable 21. Vaginal intercourse is the only legitimate type of sex 24. Foreplay is a waste of time 25. Sex is meant only for procreation 26. In sex, the quickest/faster the best 32. There is only one acceptable way of having sex (missionary position) 33. Sexual intercourse before marriage is a sin F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F6 0.52 0.04 0.25 0.10 0.34 7 0.04 0.51 0.19 0.36 0.11 0.11 0.27 0.47 0.31 0.24 0.05 0.07 7 0.05 0.63 0.07 0.14 0.09 0.41 0.12 0.53 0.47 0.16 7 0.25 0.18 7 0.09 0.70 0.72 0.11 0.07 0.35 0.08 7 0.04 0.15 0.20 0.15 7 0.11 0.22 0.72 0.67 0.29 7 0.01 0.03 0.03 0.72 0.09 7 0.05 0.21 0.01 0.00 0.76 7 0.04 7 0.10 0.14 0.00 0.06 0.09 0.42 0.31 0.20 0.25 7 0.05 0.20 0.08 0.41 0.65 7 0.18 0.18 7 0.01 7 0.01 0.21 0.13 0.39 0.06 7 0.01 0.56 0.16 0.12 0.10 7 0.05 0.07 0.54 0.11 0.34 0.18 0.17 0.16 0.49 0.33 0.11 7 0.34 0.08 0.13 0.62 7 0.11 0.41 7 0.10 0.02 0.13 0.51 0.18 0.12 0.02 0.09 Female sexual power 11. 15. 19. 27. 29. 38. 39. 40. The consequences of a sexual failure are catastrophic In bed the woman is the boss A woman’s body is her best weapon People who don’t control their sexual urges are more easily controlled by others If a man lets himself go sexually he is under a woman’s control A real man doesn’t need much stimulation to reach orgasm A woman at her sexual peak can get whatever she wants from a man The greater the sexual intimacy, the greater the potential for getting hurt (continued ) Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 185 TABLE VI. (continued ) Factors SDBQ items F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F6 0.17 0.23 0.59 0.29 7 0.12 7 0.05 0.15 7 0.01 0.06 0.11 0.46 0.52 0.30 0.43 0.28 0.13 7 0.05 0.21 0.29 0.24 0.45 0.01 0.13 0.24 0.18 0.26 0.66 0.23 0.01 0.13 0.09 0.25 0.46 0.37 0.05 7 0.14 0.18 0.01 0.53 7 0.02 0.29 7 0.31 0.21 0.01 0.09 0.60 0.28 7 0.14 0.07 0.11 0.18 0.69 7 0.09 0.13 0.18 0.27 0.28 0.47 0.17 7 0.21 0.04 0.15 0.20 0.68 0.05 0.08 0.13 0.36 0.31 0.49 0.05 0.18 0.05 0.29 0.09 7 0.00 0.60 0.11 0.25 0.02 0.10 0.16 0.41 0.15 ‘Macho’ belief 1. 4. 6. 17. 28. 31. 37. A real man has sexual intercourse very often Homosexuality is a sickness A real man must wait the necessary amount of time to sexual satisfy a woman In sex, getting to the climax is most important A real man is always ready for sex and must be capable of satisfying any woman A man must be capable of maintaining an erection until the end of any sex Sex can be good even without orgasma Beliefs about women’s satisfaction 3. Penis erection is essential for a woman’s sexual satisfaction 7. A woman may have doubts about a man’s virility when he fails an erection 16. Men who are not capable of penetrating women can’t satisfy them 35. A man who doesn’t sexually satisfy a woman is a failure 36. Whenever a situation arises, a real man must be capable of penetration Restrictive attitude toward sex 8. Repeated engagement on oral/ anal sex can cause serious health problems 12. Women only pay attention to attractive younger man (continued ) 186 Pedro J. Nobre et al. TABLE VI. (continued ) Factors SDBQ items F1 13. 30. It is not appropriate to have sexual fantasies during sexual intercourse Anal sex is a perverted activity F2 F3 F4 F5 0.29 7 0.02 0.12 F6 7 0.04 0.20 0.52 7 0.08 0.17 0.14 7 0.02 0.75 0.04 0.21 0.15 0.38 0.38 0.15 0.08 7 0.01 0.14 0.23 0.15 0.52 7 0.53 0.39 0.06 0.02 0.21 0.15 0.53 Sex as an abuse of men’s power 10. 22. 34. a Sex is an abuse of male’s power The quality of the erection is what most satisfies women Sex is a violation of a woman’s body Items scored in reverse order. TABLE VII. Male SDBQ domain intercorrelations (n = 206) Domains F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F6 Sexual conservatism Female sexual power ‘Macho’ belief Beliefs about women’s satisfaction Restrictive attitude toward sex Sex as an abuse of men’s power F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F6 – 0.52** 0.55** 0.44** 0.59** 0.61** – 0.57** 0.53** 0.41** 0.46** – 0.62** 0.42** 0.39** – 0.34** 0.34** – 0.47* – *p 5 0.05. **p 5 0.01. TABLE VIII. Domain and total scores of the SDBQ (female version) Domains Item numbers F1 Sexual conservatism F2 Female sexual power F3 ‘Macho’ belief F4 Beliefs about women’s satisfaction F5 Restrictive attitude toward sex F6 Sex as an abuse of men’s power Total 2, 5, 9, 18, 21, 24, 25, 26, 32, 33 11, 15, 19, 27, 29, 38, 39, 40 1, 4, 6, 17, 28, 31, 37 3, 7, 16, 35, 36 8, 12, 13, 30 10, 22, 34 – Note: Item 37 is scored in reverse order. Minimum Maximum 10 8 7 5 4 3 37 50 40 35 25 20 15 185 Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 187 TABLE IX. Test – retest reliability SDBQ domains N items Pearson r 9 6 0.65** 0.23 5 4 6 4 34 0.65** 0.17 0.29 0.73** 0.80** 10 8 7 5 0.28 0.46 0.62 0.82** 4 3 0.71* 0.80** 37 0.73* Female sample (n = 26) F1 Sexual conservatism F2 Sexual desire and pleasure as a sin F3 Age related beliefs F4 Body-image beliefs F5 Denying affection primacy F6 Motherhood primacy Total Male sample (n = 10) F1 Sexual conservatism F2 Female sexual power F3 Macho belief F4 Beliefs about women’s satisfaction F5 Restrictive attitude toward sex F6 Sex as an abuse of men’s power Total *p 5 0.05. **p 5 0.01. Sexual Beliefs and Information Questionnaire (SBIQ) In an overall analysis of the correlation between our measure and the Sexual Beliefs and Information Questionnaire (SBIQ) we observed a moderate negative relationship between both total scores (r = 7 0.42, p 5 0.05 in the female sample and r = 7 0.48, p 5 0.05 for the male sample), showing that they assess somewhat similar concepts (the negative direction is due to the inverse meaning of the scores – with high scores on the SBIQ meaning degree of accurate sexual information and high scores on our measure meaning sexual dysfunctional beliefs). A more detailed look at the inter-correlations between the specific dimensions of both measures, led us to the following observations (Table XI): In the female sample, sexual conservatism and denying affection primacy are both significantly related to the Time/Patience dimension of the SBIQ (r = 7 0.32, p 5 0.05 and r = 7 0.33, p 5 0.05). This SBIQ’s domain is characterized by the idea that time and patience plays a central role in sexual satisfaction, sharing information about one’s desires and taking the time needed to please a partner. It is not surprising that this factor correlates inversely with sexual conservatism (coitus primacy and negation of other sexual activities) and denying affection primacy (negation of the affection’s role on sexual activity). Another statistically significant correlation (r = 7 0.33, p 5 0.05) is between age related beliefs and the sexual satisfaction domain of the SBIQ. The sexual satisfaction 188 Pedro J. Nobre et al. TABLE X. Internal consistency SDBQ domains N items Cronbach’s alpha 9 6 5 4 6 4 34 0.78 0.75 0.74 0.66 0.59 0.50 0.81 10 8 7 5 4 3 37 0.89 0.77 0.75 0.75 0.54 0.63 0.93 Female sample (n = 154) F1 Sexual conservatism F2 Sexual desire and pleasure as a sin F3 Age related beliefs F4 Body-image beliefs F5 Denying affection primacy F6 Motherhood primacy Total Male sample (n = 206) F1 Sexual conservatism F2 Female sexual power F3 ‘Macho’ belief F4 Beliefs about women’s satisfaction F5 Restrictive attitude toward sex F6 Sex as an abuse of men’s power Total factor is associated with correct ideas about how to sexually satisfy both males and females. Ideas about sexual decline with age, correlate inversely with that SBIQ’s factor. In the male version we may highlight the negative relation between the time/patience factor of the SBIQ and: sexual conservatism (r = 7 0.44, p 5 0.05), restrictive attitude toward sex (r = 7 0.43, p 5 0.05) and sex as an abuse of men’s power (r = 7 0.41, p 5 0.05). All these three dimensions from SDBQ are opposite to the idea of sharing one’s desires and taking the time needed to satisfy both partners. Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (DAS) In order to assess the relationship between sexual dysfunctional beliefs and more general dysfunctional beliefs, we analysed the relationship between our questionnaire and the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (DAS). This instrument was developed to evaluate general beliefs that proved to be related to several psychopathological situations (Weissman & Beck, 1978; Pinto-Gouveia et al., 1987). The idea was to assess cognitive profiles that could constitute vulnerability factors to the development of emotional disorders. We hypothesized that dysfunctional beliefs about sexuality could be related to more general dysfunctional beliefs about ourselves and others. When we look at the correlations between our questionnaire and the DAS (Table XII), there is a closed relationship among several dimensions of both scales. In the female version there are high correlations between the sexual beliefs total score and the dependence of social approval (r = 0.71, p 5 0.01) and absolutist imperatives (r = 0.63, p 5 0.01). Thus, we may conclude that our female sexual dysfunctional beliefs scale Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 189 TABLE XI. Correlations of the male and female SDBQ domains with the SBIQ total and domains SBIQ domains SDBQ domains Time patience Stress pressure Ageing Sexual satisfaction Basic knowledge 7 0.32* 7 0.23 7 0.28 0.06 0.09 0.08 0.18 7 0.13 7 0.34* 7 0.18 7 0.20 0.07 7 0.19 0.03 0.26 7 0.33* 7 0.14 7 0.06 7 0.23 7 0.27 7 0.01 7 0.27 7 0.14 7 0.05 7 0.09 7 0.34 7 0.44* 7 0.04 7 0.37 7 0.24 7 0.10 0.23 7 0.02 7 0.11 7 0.09 7 0.08 0.22 7 0.12 7 0.65** 7 0.01 7 0.39 7 0.33 7 0.31 7 0.07 7 0.5 7 0.39 7 0.66** 7 0.00 7 0.32 7 0.53* 7 0.43* 7 0.17 0.24 0.02 7 0.10 7 0.11 7 0.41* 7 0.17 0.11 7 0.19 7 0.23 7 0.27 7 0.44* 0.01 0.01 7 0.40 7 0.28 7 0.48* Total Female sample (n = 45) Sexual conservatism Sexual desire/ pleasure as a sin Age related beliefs Body-image beliefs Denying affection primacy Motherhood primacy Total 7 0.28 7 0.13 0.11 0.17 7 0.20 0.05 7 0.09 0.07 0.09 7 0.37* 7 0.05 7 0.27 7 0.24 7 0.18 7 0.26 7 0.42* Male sample (n = 23) Sexual conservatism Female sexual power ‘Macho’ belief Beliefs about women’s satisfaction Restrictive attitude toward sex Sex as an abuse of men’s power Total *p 5 0.05. **p 5 0.01. assesses general beliefs of dependence of approval by others and absolutist imperatives. Apart from the close relationship that is common across several dimensions (sexual conservatism, sex as a sin, age related beliefs, body image beliefs and motherhood primacy), we may also highlight the correlations between perfectionism and fear of rejection from the DAS and: sex as a sin (r = 0.44, p 5 0.05), and motherhood primacy (r = 0.50, p 5 0.05). This could mean that behind the idea of sex as a sin and the primacy of motherhood pleasures over sexual enjoyment, there is a fear of social rejection and a tendency to be perfectionist in order to prevent social criticism. In the same direction, we observed that the adaptive coping domain of the DAS is negatively related to sex as a sin (r = 7 0.41, p 5 0.05) and age related beliefs (r = 7 0.49, p 5 0.05). This means that females with beliefs of sex as a sin or that view ageing in opposition of sexual expression are less capable to cope with failure situations. When we analyse the male version, it is clear that there is an even stronger relationship between the DAS and sexual dysfunctional beliefs. SDBQ total score presents high 190 Pedro J. Nobre et al. correlations with perfectionism /rejection fear (r = 0.74, p 5 0.01), dependence of social approval (r = 0.66, p 5 0.01), absolutist/imperative thinking (r = 0.60, p 5 0.01), performance perfectionism (r = 0.58, p 5 0.01) and personal autonomy (r = 0.40, p 5 0.05). We can postulate that male sexual beliefs are not only related to more general beliefs of dependence of social approval and absolutist thinking, but also to a more perfectionism related dimension (both work and social approval themes). Sexual Self-Schema (SSS) Another instrument used to assess convergent validity was the Sexual Self-Schema (SSS, form F-female and M-male). This questionnaire was specifically developed to assess both male and female ideas about oneself as a sexual being (sexual self-view). This measure is composed of three specific domains in both male and female forms: Passionate/Romantic which is common for the two forms, Open/Direct and Conservative for the female version and, Powerful/Aggressive and Liberal for the male form. These dimensions, therefore, formed the aspects assessed as sexual self-views for both males and females. Correlations between our measures and the SSS show that in general, our female’s version total score present a moderate inverse relationship (r = 7 0.32, p 5 0.05) with the Open/Direct dimension of the SSS female form (Table XIII). Thus, females with high sexual dysfunctional beliefs do not present themselves as sexually open or direct persons. When we look carefully at the specific dimensions of both instruments, we observe that there are few statistically significant correlations between the assessed domains. The exception is the sexual conservatism factor of our questionnaire which as expected, correlate highly with the open/direct (r = 7 0.46, p 5 0.01) and the conservative dimensions (r = 0.31, p 5 0.05) of the SSS. When we look at the male version, we observe a moderate relationship between the liberal dimension of the SSS and sexual conservatism (r = 7 0.55, p 5 0.05), and ‘macho’ beliefs (r = 7 0.53, p = 0.05) dimensions of our measure. As in the female version, the higher the male’s sexual dysfunctional beliefs (specifically conservative and macho) the smaller the tendency to present themselves as sexually liberal subjects. This means that there is a convergence between the self-concept of sexual conservative/liberal and the beliefs we present about sexuality. In an overall analysis, we may conclude, that despite the relationship between the conservative/liberal vector of the sexual self-view and some consistent sexual beliefs (conservative in nature), none of the other dimensions of the SSS are linked to the proposed dysfunctional sexual beliefs. The self-views of being sexually romantic or powerful and aggressive are not related to any of the dysfunctional sexual beliefs presented. Male and Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI, IIEF) Regarding the correlation between the sexual dysfunctional beliefs questionnaire and both male and female sexual function, we found several statistically significant correlations. This shows that our beliefs about sexuality are somehow related to our sexual functioning (Table XIV). TABLE XII. Correlations of the male and female SDBQ domains with DAS total and domains DAS Domains SDBQ domains Dependence of social approval Adaptive cognitions High performance demands Adaptive coping Autonomy Total Perfectionism Absolutist imperatives 0.19 0.44* 0.17 0.38 0.10 0.43* 0.41* 0.70** 0.50* 0.00 0.46* 0.35 0.70** 0.43* 7 0.20 7 0.20 7 0.12 7 0.16 7 0.03 7 0.22 7 0.15 7 0.05 0.26 7 0.03 0.09 7 0.30 7 0.41* 7 0.49* 7 0.27 0.50* 7 0.09 7 0.09 7 0.11 7 0.06 0.23 0.17 0.30 0.46* 0.34 0.07 0.51* 0.40 0.45* 0.63** 0.55** 0.71** 7 0.37 7 0.18 7 0.00 7 0.10 7 0.17 7 0.40 0.29 7 0.02 0.38 0.38 0.70** 0.63** 0.61** 0.61** 0.14 0.57** 0.42* 0.33 0.45* 0.53** 0.40* 0.53** 7 0.43* 0.01 7 0.22 7 0.19 0.54** 0.51** 0.53** 0.48** 7 0.08 7 0.04 0.13 7 0.23 0.47* 0.55** 0.74** 0.27 0.39* 0.60** 0.27 0.33 0.66** 7 0.35 7 0.40 7 0.06 0.13 0.35 0.58** 0.09 7 0.03 7 0.11 Female sample (n = 25) Male sample (n = 32) Sexual conservatism Female sexual power Macho belief Women’s satisfaction belief Restrictive sex attitude Sex as men’s abuse Total *p 5 0.05. **p 5 0.01. 0.15 0.28 0.11 0.49** 7 0.10 7 0.02 0.40* 0.51** 0.59** 0.50** 0.60** 0.21 0.38 0.68** Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire Sexual conservatism Sexual desire as a sin Age related beliefs Body-image beliefs Denying affection primacy Motherhood primacy Total 191 192 Pedro J. Nobre et al. TABLE XIII. Correlations of the male and female SDBQ domains with SSS total and domains SSS domains SDBQ domains Passionate/Romantic Open/Direct Conservative Female sample (n = 50) Sexual conservatism Sexual desire/pleasure as a sin Age related beliefs Body-image beliefs Denying affection primacy Motherhood primacy Total 7 0.04 7 0.19 0.01 0.04 7 0.17 7 0.04 7 0.13 7 0.46** 7 0.27 0.02 0.02 0.07 7 0.17 7 0.32* 0.31* 0.01 0.08 0.05 7 0.14 0.02 0.17 Male sample (n = 15) SSS domains SDBQ domains Sexual conservatism Female sexual power ‘Macho’ belief Beliefs about women’s satisfaction Restrictive attitude toward sex Sex as an abuse of men’s power Total Passionate/Loving Powerful/Aggressive 0.19 7 0.48 0.08 7 0.27 0.27 7 0.01 7 0.17 7 0.33 7 0.31 7 0.24 7 0.20 7 0.26 7 0.12 7 0.41 Liberal 7 0.55* 7 0.31 7 0.53 7 0.15 7 0.50 0.00 7 0.45 *p 5 0.05. **p 5 0.01. Looking at the female sample, the FSFI total score presented high negative correlations with: sexual conservatism (r = 7 0.35, p 5 0.01), sex as a sin (r = 7 0.32, p 5 0.01), age related beliefs (r = 7 0.33, p 5 0.01) and body image beliefs (r = 7 0.24, p 5 0.01). Regarding the male sample, there were high negative correlations between the IIEF total scores and: sexual conservatism (r = 7 0.40, p 5 0.01), beliefs about women’s satisfaction (r = 7 0.39, p 5 0.001), ‘macho’ belief (r = 7 0.32, p 5 0.01), restrictive attitude toward sex (r = 7 0.28, p 5 0.001) and female sexual power (r = 7 0.26, p 5 0.01). Discriminant validity In order to analyse discriminant validity, we used t-test of mean differences between a clinical group and a control group of community volunteers (demographic data presented in Table II). We hypothesized that dysfunctional subjects would present higher scores on both male and female sexual dysfunctional beliefs, supporting the hypothesis that our questionnaires assess sexual beliefs that could represent a vulnerability factor for developing sexual problems. Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 193 Results for both male and female populations, although not statistically significant, showed that subjects from the clinical group presented higher results on several domains of the SDBQ compared to the control subjects (Table XV, Figure 1 and 2). Female dysfunctional group present higher scores in sexual conservatism (t = 0.96, p = 0.34), sex as a sin (t = 1.52, p = 0.13) and body-image beliefs (t = 0.98, p = 0.33), while dysfunctional males showed higher scores on ‘macho’ belief (t = 1.47, p = 0.15). Discussion and Conclusions The purpose of this study was to develop a measure of male and female sexual beliefs that could be related to sexual disorders. Starting from a cognitivist point of view, we hypothesised that sexual disorders are characterised by a set of beliefs about sexuality, that could play a central role as predisposing factors of sexual dysfunction. These beliefs or ideas about sexuality and sexual expression would guide our sexual behaviour, and would be used in interpretation for sexual events. Thus, if a man presents a belief that he must maintain an erection until the end of any sexual situation, he would tend to interpret any decrease of erectile level in a sexual context as a failure (possible developing more erectile difficulties). In addition, if a woman believed that vaginal coitus is the only acceptable way of having sex, she could present some difficulties in experiencing orgasm. These simple examples show how specific sexual beliefs could affect sexual expression. Using several beliefs and attitudes proposed as etiologic factors of sexual dysfunction (Hawton, 1985; Kaplan, 1979; Lazarus, 1988; Lo Piccolo & Friedman, 1988; Masters & Johnson, 1970; Rosen & Leiblum, 1995; Tevlin & Leiblum, 1983; Wincze & Barlow, 1997; Zilbergeld, 1992), we developed a 40 item male and female questionnaire, after a careful item analysis of a primary pool of 95 questions. Both male and female forms presented high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.93 for the male and .81 for the female version) as well as acceptable test – retest reliability results (r = 0.73 and r = 0.80 respectively). A factor analysis of both forms was performed to study internal structure, showing that these two versions present a six factor structure: sexual conservatism, sexual desire and pleasure as a sin, age related beliefs, body image beliefs, denying affection primacy and motherhood primacy for the female version; and: sexual conservatism, female sexual power, ‘macho’ belief, beliefs about women’s satisfaction beliefs, restrictive attitude toward sex and sex as an abuse of men’s power for male version. The inter-correlations between the several dimensions of both measures showed the consistency of the questionnaires with statistically high correlations across almost all factors. Regarding validity studies, we analysed the relationship between our instrument and other measures of sexual or more general beliefs. The SBIQ, a similar measure of sexual information and beliefs, was related to our questionnaires, especially in the time/ patience dimension of the SBIQ (characterized by the idea that time and sharing plays a central role in sexual satisfaction). Results indicate that the greater the dysfunctional sexual beliefs, the greater the tendency to deny sharing information about one’s desire and taking the time needed to please the partner. 194 TABLE XIV. Correlations of the male and female automatic thoughts sub-scale domains with the FSFI and IIEF domains and total SDBQ domains Desire Arousal Lubrication Orgasm Satisfaction Pain Total 7 0.40** 7 0.36** 7 0.32** 7 0.28** 7 0.05 7 0.17* 7 0.35** 7 0.30** 7 0.25** 7 0.19* 7 0.22** 7 0.07 7 0.17* 7 0.31** 7 0.21** 7 0.17* 7 0.10 7 0.12 7 0.09 7 0.14 7 0.18* 7 0.18* 7 0.24** 7 0.17* 7 0.20** 7 0.05 7 0.15* 7 0.25** 7 0.33** 7 0.24** 7 0.39** 7 0.20** 7 0.03 7 0.10 7 0.32** 7 0.21** 7 0.07 7 0.16* 7 0.00 7 0.00 7 0.13 7 0.17* 7 0.35** 7 0.32** 7 0.33** 7 0.24** 7 0.07 7 0.14 7 0.36** Female sample (n = 154) Sexual conservatism Sexual desire/pleasure as a sin Age related beliefs Body-image beliefs Denying affection primacy Motherhood primacy Total Male sample (n = 163) IIEF domains SDBQ domains Sexual conservatism Female sexual power ‘Macho’ belief Beliefs about women’s satisfaction Restrictive attitude toward sex Sex as an abuse of men’s power Total *p 5 0.05. **p 5 0.01. Desire Erection Orgasm Overall satisfaction Intercourse satisfaction Total 7 0.20** 7 0.13 7 0.14* 7 0.15* 7 0.19** 7 0.10 7 0.24* 7 0.36** 7 0.25** 7 0.32** 7 0.37** 7 0.25** 7 0.11 7 0.44** 7 0.30** 7 0.25** 7 0.25** 7 0.30** 7 0.20** 7 0.07 7 0.31** 7 0.32** 7 0.18** 7 0.20** 7 0.29** 7 0.19 7 0.03 7 0.34** 7 0.28** 7 0.23** 7 0.28** 7 0.29** 7 0.16* 0.01 7 0.39** 7 0.40** 7 0.26** 7 0.32** 7 0.39** 7 0.28** 7 0.11 7 0.44** Pedro J. Nobre et al. FSFI domains 195 Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire TABLE XV. Means and standard deviations of male and female SDBQ domains in a clinical and control group and between groups t-test Female sample Clinical group (n = 47) Control group (n = 46) SDBQ domains Mean SD Mean SD t Sexual conservatism Sexual desire/pleasure as a sin Age related beliefs Body-image beliefs Denying affection primacy Motherhood primacy Total 16.51 8.52 10.20 6.34 9.10 7.53 57.68 6.23 3.94 3.56 3.09 3.55 2.64 15.93 15.11 7.47 9.95 5.75 9.29 7.50 52.00 7.05 2.38 4.17 2.63 3.42 2.30 13.95 0.96 1.52 0.30 0.98 7 0.25 0.06 1.59 Male sample Clinical group (n = 49) Control group (n = 49) SDBQ domains Mean SD Mean SD t Sexual conservatism Female sexual power ‘Macho’ belief Beliefs about women’s satisfaction Restrictive attitude toward sex Sex as an abuse of men’s power Total 19.72 22.21 22.89 15.96 10.23 3.48 92.88 7.99 5.49 6.28 4.82 2.93 1.69 21.49 19.62 22.93 21.04 14.89 11.35 3.85 90.26 8.63 5.16 6.00 4.55 3.45 2.20 23.99 0.06 7 0.63 1.47 1.09 7 1.68 7 0.91 0.50 The Sexual Self-Schema, a questionnaire assessing self-views in sexual context is another measure partially related to our instruments. The conservative/liberal vector of the SSS has shown high correlations with both male and female sexual conservative beliefs and the male ‘macho’ belief. This supports that there is a consistency between sexual beliefs (especially the conservative ones) and subject’s self-concept as sexual conservative persons. In order to analyse the relationship between sexual beliefs and more generic beliefs of ourselves and others, we studied the relationship with the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (DAS). These general beliefs accepted as predisposing factors of several psychopathological situations also prove to be highly related to several dimensions of both male and female sexual beliefs. This is especially evident for dependence of social approval, absolutist imperatives and perfectionism. We may conclude, that behind sexual dysfunctional beliefs, could be more general cognitive profiles that have played an important role in the development of different psychopathological situations, especially depression (Weissman & Beck, 1978; Pinto-Gouveia et al., 1987). Finally, with the purpose of studying the impact of sexual dysfunctional beliefs on sexual disorders, we assessed the relationship between our measure and male and 196 Pedro J. Nobre et al. FIG. 1. Mean scores of functional and dysfunctional groups in SDBQ domains (female version). FIG. 2. Mean scores of functional and dysfunctional groups in SDBQ domains (male version). female sexual functioning scales, and performed a discriminant analysis, evaluating its capacity to distinguish between a clinical (sexual dysfunctional) and a control group (sexual functional). Both analyses supported our hypothesis that sexual dysfunctional beliefs are somehow related with sexual dysfunction. Several domains from both male and female SDBQ showed statistically significant negative correlations with the sexual function scores. Discriminant analysis between a clinical and a control group although not showing statistically significant differences somehow supported the findings from the correlational studies. The less significant results from this analysis could be related Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire 197 with the relative small number of subjects used. Further studies with larger samples must be conducted in order to better clarify this issue (now in progress). In general, our results seems to validate the idea that some beliefs about sexuality could be related to dysfunctional manifestations, supporting some theoretical conceptualisations based on clinical observations. Religious beliefs and conservatism (Kaplan, 1979; LoPiccolo & Friedman, 1988; Masters & Johnson, 1970), body-image beliefs (LoPiccolo & Friedman, 1988; Rosen & Leiblum, 1995) and lack of information regarding sexuality (Hawton, 1985) proved to be significantly higher in our female clinical sample. In addition, high performance beliefs, beliefs about women’s sexual satisfaction, and sexual conservatism (Hawton, 1985; Wincze & Barlow, 1997; Zilbergeld, 1992, 1999), were higher in our male clinical sample in comparison withthe non-clinical group. Thus, these sexual beliefs maybe conceptualised as cognitive vulnerabilities to sexual dysfunction. Using a cognitive perspective, the presence of sexual dysfunctional beliefs stipulating a range of conditions for the activation of negative schemas, constitute a predisposing factor for the development of sexual difficulties. The sexual dysfunctional beliefs questionnaire is a valid, reliable and important measure for assessing ideas about sexuality that play a major role in the development of sexual disorders. Although more consistent and larger studies both with clinical and non-clinical populations are needed (now in progress), we think that SDBQ may be useful in both clinical practice and educational programs as an indicator of vulnerability to sexual dysfunction. Acknowledgements This research was partially supported by a grant from PRODEP. The authors would like to thank to D. Rijo, MA, C. Salvador, MA, M. Lima, PhD, Faculdade de Psicologia, Universidade de Coimbra, Portugal; A. Gomes, MA, L. Fonseca, MA, A. Carvalheira, MA, J. Teixeira, MD, G. Santos, MD, J. Quartilho, MD, PhD., P. Abrantes, MD, A. Canhao, MD, Hospitais da Universidade de Coimbra, for their suggestions and help in sample collection. H. Ramsawh, MA , L. Scepkowski, MA and M. Santos, BA, Center for Anxiety and Related Disorders, Boston University for reviewing the English version of the measures. John Wincze, PhD, Brown University and Center for Anxiety and Related Disorders, Boston University, for his review and suggestions on a previous version of the paper. Thanks also to participants who volunteered to participate in the study. References ADAMS, S.G., DUBBERT, P.M., CHUPURDIA, K.M., JONES, A., LOFLAND, K.R. & LEERMAKERS, E. (1996). Assessment of sexual beliefs and information in aging couples with sexual dysfunction. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 25, 249 – 260. ANDERSEN, B.L. & CYRANOWSKI, J.M. (1994). Women’s sexual self-schema. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1079 – 1100. ANDERSEN, B.L., CYRANOWSKI, J.M. & ESPINDLE, D. (1999). Men’s sexual self-schema. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 645 – 61. BAKER, C. & DE SILVA, P. (1988). The relationship between male sexual dysfunction and belief in Zilbergeld’s myths: an empirical investigation. Sexual and Marital Therapy, 3, 229 – 238. 198 Pedro J. Nobre et al. BARNETT, P.A. & GOTLIB, I.H. (1988). Psychosocial functioning and depression: Distinguish among antecedents, concomitants, and consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 97 – 126. BECK, A.T. (1988). Love is never enough. New York: Harper & Raw. BECK, A.T. (1996). Beyond belief: A theory of modes, personality, and psychopathology. In P. SALKOVSKIS, (Ed), Frontiers of cognitive therapy (pp. 1 – 25). New York: Guilford Press. BECK, A.T. (1999). Prisoners of hate: The cognitive basis of anger, hostility and violence. New York: Guilford. BECK, A.T. & EMERY, G. (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. New York: Basic Books. BECK, A.T., EPSTEIN, N., HARRISON, R.P. & EMERY, G. (1983). Development of the sociotropy-autonomy scale: A measure of personality factors in psychopathology. Unpublished manuscript, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia. BECK, A.T. & FREEMAN, A.T. (1990). Cognitive therapy of personality disorders. New York: Guilford Press. BECK, A.T., RUSH, A.J., SHAW, B.F. & EMERY, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press. BECK, A.T., WRIGHT, F.D., NEWMAN, C.F. & LIESE, B.S. (1993). Cognitive therapy of substance abuse. New York: Guilford Press. BIELING, P.J., BECK, A.T. & BROWN, G.K. (2000). The sociotropy-autonomy scale: structure and implications. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 763 – 780. CAMPBELL, D.T. & FISKE, D.W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81 – 105. CARDOSO, F.M. (1998). Depressão e dimensões de personalidade de sociotropia/autonomia. [Depression and personality dimensions of sociotropy/autonomy]. Unpublished master’s thesis. Faculdade de Psicologia e Ciências da Educação da Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal. CLARK, D.A. & BECK, A.T. (1991). Personality factors in dysphoria: A psychometric refinement of Beck’s sociotropy-autonomy scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 13, 369 – 388. CRONBACH, L.J. & MEEHL, P.E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281 – 302. HAWTON, K. (1985). Sex therapy: A practical guide. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson Inc. KAPLAN, H. (1979). Disorders of sexual desire: and other new concepts and techniques in sex therapy. New York: Brunner Mazel. LAZARUS, A. (1988). A multimodal perspective on problems of sexual desire. In S. LEIBLUM & R. ROSEN, (Eds), Sexual desire disorders (pp. 145 – 167). New York: Guilford Press. LOPICCOLO, J. & FRIEDMAN, J. (1988). Broad spectrum treatment of low sexual desire: Integration of cognitive, behavioral and systemic therapy. In S. LEIBLUM & R. ROSEN, (Eds), Sexual desire disorders (pp. 107 – 144). New York: Guilford Press. MASTERS, W. & JOHNSON, V. (1970). Human sexual inadequacy. Boston: Little Brown & Co. NOBRE, P.J. (1997). Disfunção Eréctil: contributos para a construção de um modelo cognitivo-afectivo. (Erectile Disorder: contributes for the development of a cognitive-affective model). Unpublished master’s thesis. Faculdade de Psicologia e Ciências da Educação da Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal. NOBRE, P.J. (1999). Factores cognitivo-emocionais da disfuncao sexual. (Cognitive-emotional factors of sexual dysfunction). Unpublished Ph.D. research project, Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal. NOBRE, P.J. (2000a). The Sexual Beliefs and Information Questionnaire: Portuguese version. Unpublished manuscript. Faculdade de Psicologia e Ciências da Educação da Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal. NOBRE, P.J. (2000b). The Sexual Self-Schema: Portuguese version. Unpublished manuscript. Faculdade de Psicologia e Ciências da Educação da Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal. NOBRE, P.J. & PINTO-GOUVEIA, J. (2000a). Erectile dysfunction: An empirical approach based on Beck’s cognitive theory. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 15, 351 – 366. NOBRE, P.J. & PINTO-GOUVEIA, J. (2002b). Cognitive schema activation in sexual context. A questionnaire to assess cognitive schemas activated in sexual failure situations.Manuscript submitted for publication. NOBRE, P.J. & PINTO-GOUVEIA, J. (2002c). Sexual modal questionnaire: Measure to assess the interaction between cognitions, emotions and sexual response. Manuscript submitted for publication. PINTO-GOUVEIA, J., VAZ-SERRA, A., ROBALO, M., FONSECA, L. & MATOS, A. (1987). Dysfunctional attitudes and self-concept. In J.P. DAUWALDER, M. PEREZ & V. HOBI, (Eds), Controversial issues in behaviour modification. Annual Series of European Research in Behaviour Therapy, Vol 2., (p. 305). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger. 199 Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire ROSEN, R.C., BROWN, C., HEIMAN, J., LEIBLUM, S., MESTON, C., SHABSIG, R., FERGUNSON, D. & D’AGOSTINO, R. (2000). The female sexual function index (FSFI): A multidimensional self-report instrument for the assessment of female sexual function. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 26, 191 – 208. ROSEN, R.C. & LEIBLUM, S.R. (1995). The changing focus of sex therapy. In R. ROSEN & S. LEIBLUM, (Eds), Case studies in sex therapy (pp. 3 – 17). New York: Guilford Press. ROSEN, R.C., RILEY, A., WAGNER, G., OSTERLOH, I.H., KIRKPATRCK, J. & MISHRA, A. (1997). The international index of erectile function (IIEF): A multidimensional scale for assessment of erectile dysfunction. Urology, 49, 822 – 830. TEVLIN, H. & LEIBLUM, S.R. (1983). Sex role stereotypes and female sexual dysfunction. In V. FRANKS & E. ROTHBAUM, (Eds), The stereotyping of women: Its effects on mental health. New York: Springer. WEISSMAN, A. & BECK, A. (1978). The Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale. Unpublished manuscript. WINCZE, J.P. & BARLOW, D.H. (1997). Enhancing sexuality: A problem solving approach. Client work-book. Santo Antonio, TX: Graywind Publications. YOUNG, J. & BROWN, G. (1989). Schema questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript. Cognitive Therapy Center of New York. ZILBERGELD, B. (1983). Men and sex. London: Fontana. ZILBERGELD, B. (1992). The new male sexuality. New York: Bantam Books. ZILBERGELD, B. (1999). The new male sexuality (rev. edn.). New York: Bantam Books. Contributors PEDRO J. NOBRE, MSc, Clinical Psychologist JOSÉ PINTO GOUVEIA, Associate Professor and Head of Cognitive Behaviour Department FRANCISCO ALLEN GOMES, Hospitais da Universidade de Coimbra Appendix Sexual Beliefs Questionnaire (Male Version) The list presented below contains statements related to sexuality. Please read each statement carefully and circle the number in the right hand column which correspond to the extent to which you agree or disagree with each statement (circle only one option per statement—from 1— completely disagree to 5—completely agree). There are no wrong or right answers, but it is very important that you be honest and that you answer all items. . Sexual beliefs 1. 2. 3. A real man has sexual intercourse very often Orgasm is possible only by vaginal intercourse Penile erection is essential for a woman’s sexual satisfaction Disagree Don’t disagree or agree Agree Completely agree 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Completely disagree (continued ) 200 Pedro J. Nobre et al. . (continued ) Sexual beliefs 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. Homosexuality is a sickness A woman has no other choice but to be sexually subjugated by a man’s power A real man must wait the necessary amount of time to sexually satisfy a woman during intercourse A woman may have doubts about a man’s virility when he fails to get an erection during sexual activity Repeated engagement in oral or anal sex can cause serious health problems A shorter duration of intercourse is a sign of a man’s power Sex is an abuse of a male’s power The consequences of a sexual failure are catastrophic Women only pay attention to attractive younger men It is not appropriate to have sexual fantasies during sexual intercourse There are certain universal rules about what is normal during sexual activity In bed the woman is the boss Men who are not capable of penetrating women can’t satisfy them sexually In sex, getting to the climax is most important In sex anything but vaginal intercourse is unacceptable A woman’s body is her best weapon A woman may stop loving a man if he his not capable of satisfying her sexually Vaginal intercourse is the only legitimate type of sex The quality of the erection is what most satisfies women Disagree Don’t disagree or agree Agree Completely agree 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Completely disagree (continued ) 201 Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire . (continued ) Sexual beliefs 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. A successful career implies the control of sexual urges Foreplay is a waste of time Sex is meant only for procreation In sex, the quicker/faster the better People who don’t control their sexual urges are more easily controlled by others A real man is always ready for sex and must be capable of satisfying any woman If a man lets himself go sexually he is under a woman’s control Anal sex is a perverted activity A man must be capable of maintaining an erection until the end of any sexual activity There is only one acceptable way of having sex (missionary position) Sexual intercourse before marriage is a sin Sex is a violation of a woman’s body A man who doesn’t sexually satisfy a woman is a failure Whenever the situation arises, a real man must be capable of penetration Sex can be good even without orgasm A real man doesn’t need much stimulation to reach orgasm A woman at her sexual peak can get whatever she wants from a man The greater the sexual intimacy, the greater the potential for getting hurt Disagree Don’t disagree or agree Agree Completely agree 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Completely disagree 202 Pedro J. Nobre et al. Sexual Beliefs Questionnaire (Female Version) The list presented below contains statements related to sexuality. Please read each statement carefully and circle the number in the right hand column which correspond to the extent to which you agree or disagree with each statement (circle only one option per statement—from 1— completely disagree to 5—completely disagree). There are no wrong or right answers, but it is very important that you be honest and that you answer all items. . Sexual beliefs 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. Love and affection from a partner are necessary for good sex Masturbation is wrong and sinful The most important component of sex is mutual affection The best gift a woman could bring to marriage is her virginity After menopause women lose their sexual desire Women who have sexual fantasies are perverted Masturbation is not a proper activity for respectable women After menopause women can’t reach orgasm There are a variety of ways of getting pleasure and reaching orgasm Women who are not physically attractive can’t be sexually satisfied In the bedroom the man is the boss A good mother can’t be sexually active Reaching climax /orgasm is acceptable for men but not for women Sexual activity must be initiated by the man Sex is dirty and sinful Disagree Don’t disagree or agree Agree Completely agree 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Completely disagree (continued ) 203 Sexual Dysfunctional Beliefs Questionnaire . (continued ) Sexual beliefs 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. Simultaneous orgasm for two partners is essential for a satisfying sexual encounter Orgasm is possible only by vaginal intercourse The goal of sex is for men to be satisfied A successfully professional career implies control of sexual behavior As women age the pleasure they get from sex decreases Men only pay attention to young, attractive women Sex is a beautiful and pure activity Sex without love is like food without flavor As long as both partners consent, anything goes Any woman who initiates sexual activity is immoral Sex is meant only for procreation Sexual intercourse during menstruation can cause health problems Oral sex is one of the biggest perversions If women let themselves go sexually they are totally under men’s control Being nice and smiling at men can be dangerous The most wonderful emotions that a woman can experience are maternal feelings Anal sex is a perverted activity In the bedroom the woman is the boss Sex should happen only if a man initiates There is just one acceptable way of having sex (missionary position) Disagree Don’t disagree or agree Agree Completely agree 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 5 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Completely disagree (continued ) 204 Pedro J. Nobre et al. . (continued ) Sexual beliefs 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. Experiencing pleasure during sexual intercourse is not acceptable in an virtuous woman A good mother must control her sexual urges An ugly woman is not capable of sexually satisfying her partner A woman who only derives sexual pleasure through clitoral stimulation is sick or perverted Pure girls don’t engage in sexual activity Disagree Don’t disagree or agree Agree Completely agree 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Completely disagree